Louisiana State University Louisiana State University
LSU Scholarly Repository LSU Scholarly Repository
Honors Theses Ogden Honors College
4-2020
The relationships between extracurricular activities, rehearsal, and The relationships between extracurricular activities, rehearsal, and
short-term memory recall in children short-term memory recall in children
Scarlett Hammond
Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.lsu.edu/honors_etd
Part of the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Hammond, Scarlett, "The relationships between extracurricular activities, rehearsal, and short-term
memory recall in children" (2020).
Honors Theses
. 628.
https://repository.lsu.edu/honors_etd/628
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Ogden Honors College at LSU Scholarly Repository. It
has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Scholarly Repository. For
more information, please contact [email protected].
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 1
The relationships between extracurricular activities, rehearsal, and
short-term memory recall in children
by
Scarlett Hammond
Undergraduate honors thesis under the direction of
Dr. Emily Elliott
Department of Psychology
Submitted to the LSU Roger Hadfield Ogden Honors College in partial fulfillment of
the Upper Division Honors Program.
April 2020
Louisiana State University
& Agricultural and Mechanical College
Baton Rouge, Louisiana
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 2
Abstract
Flavell, Beach and Chinsky (1966) completed a pioneer study on the evolution of spontaneous
verbal rehearsal in memory tasks as children go through formal schooling. They determined that,
as children progress through developmental stages, they begin to verbalize more during memory
tasks to remember information presented to them. This verbalization is one “rehearsal strategy”
that children develop. Miller, McCulloch, and Jarrold (2015) found that it is possible to teach
children rehearsal strategies through “rehearsal training” which in turn improved overall recall in
comparison to interactive imagery training. Rehearsal training can be seen in a variety of
activities, especially extracurricular activities such as sports or dancing that may involve the
utilization of rehearsal strategies. This study looked at the possible correlations between
extracurricular activities, rehearsal, and short-term recall in elementary school children ages 5 to
11. Participants completed a recall task that evaluated rehearsal strategies and their guardian
filled out a survey regarding the participant’s extracurricular activities. Results showed a
significant positive correlation between extracurricular hours and serial position scores. There
was a nonsignificant negative correlation between extracurricular hours and verbalization and
between verbalization and serial position scores. These findings indicate that increased
extracurricular activity hours may contribute to better performance on short-term memory recall
performance. Additionally, the findings indicate that verbalization does not have an effect on
serial position scores and is not affected by extracurricular hours.
Keywords: extracurricular, rehearsal, recall, short-term memory
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 3
In the age of technology, children are going outside less and less during the day, meaning
that their levels of physical activity have decreased substantially. It is important to consider the
effects that increasing a child’s physical activity can have on their cognitive abilities. Previous
studies have shown that as children progress developmentally, they tend to remember
information better through the acquisition of memorization techniques such as verbalization
(Flavell, Beach, & Chinsky, 1966). There have also been studies that show increased physical
activity can help improve these cognitive processes and academic performance (Kamijo,
Pontifex, O’Leary, Scudder, Wu, Castelli, & Hillman, 2011). However, few studies thus far have
examined the effect that increased physical activity, specifically in extracurricular activities that
utilize routine, can have on memorization.
Rehearsal in Children
Working memory refers to “the ability to store and manipulate information over brief
periods of time” (Miller et al., 2015, p. 1). Common tasks for measuring working memory
involves the serial ordering of some information and a distraction task to see how well a
participant can remember information after faced with a disruption, something this study will be
utilizing. A person’s performance on working memory tasks has been associated with predictions
to academic performance, intelligence, and classroom behavior (Miller et al., 2015). Although
working memory differs from short-term memory, the two have a close relationship; short-term
memory is considered to focus on just storage of information, while working memory is a
process for storing and manipulating information into your memory. For my study I am using the
term short-term memory recall (STM) because the delay period, which will be explained further
on in the methods section of this paper, does not contain a distraction task.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 4
In their experiment, Miller et al. (2015) looked at different rehearsal strategies that
children used to remember information, and trained participants in those strategies to determine
how recall was affected. A rehearsal strategy is something used to remember information as it is
being presented. The three types of training used in the experiment were cumulative rehearsal,
interactive imagery, and passive labeling. Also known as cumulative sub-vocal rehearsal,
cumulative rehearsal is when someone repeats a sequence in their head over and over in the
correct serial order. For interactive imagery, the children were instructed to visualize each of the
objects they were shown and to imagine those objects “being joined together and interacting with
one another” (Miller et al., 2015, p. 2). The final rehearsal strategy was passive labeling, where
participants were instructed to only name the item they just saw to themselves, differing from
cumulative rehearsal where they named all the objects in serial order repetitively.
The results from Miller et al. (2015) showed that participants in the rehearsal groups
performed better in terms of recall than those in the control group (passive labeling) no matter
what age. Additionally, the three groups of participants, which were also classified into two
different age groups, were matched by their initial level of performance on the verbal short-term
memory task. This finding is interesting considering there is a widely-held believe that younger
individuals are “either unable to rehearse, or show impoverished verbal serial recall because they
do not spontaneously engage in rehearsal” (Miller et al., 2015, p. 1). This idea, that as children
advance in age they being to rehearse more, is something Flavell et al. (1966) looked at in their
seminal experiment.
Flavell et al. (1966) discovered in their study of children ages 5 to 11 that, as they progressed
in age, children tended to display more verbalization (i.e. rehearsal) in memory tasks. This study
looked at how children verbalize and examined two different hypotheses that may explain a
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 5
deficiency that younger children experience in “verbal mediated performance”. The two
hypotheses, known as mediational-deficiency and production-deficiency, were originally
classified as one until it was realized that there needed to be further specification on the topic.
The new “mediational-deficiency” hypothesis that Flavell et al. (1966) used states that children
are able to produce verbalizations when they should, but these verbalizations do not mediate like
they should, making them useless. The “production-deficiency” hypothesis, on the other hand,
states that a child may know the appropriate words for the situation, but they fail to produce the
words verbally.
The purpose of the Flavell et al. (1966) was to examine this second hypothesis, the
production-deficiency hypothesis. To do so, they gathered a group of children ages 5 11 as
participants and administered a task to test their memory recall and verbalization abilities. This
task was replicated in this current study, and involved children being presented with a set of
pictures and then being asked to either immediately recall the pictures and the order they were
shown or experience a delay of approximately fifteen seconds before being asked to recall the
pictures and the order they were shown. Participants in this study were watched by an
experimenter who judged the amount of verbalization a child produced and classified it as no
verbalization or verbalization that did not constitute labeling, verbalization that was not
completely intelligible but could be understood as labeling, and verbalization that was clearly
labeling. Verbalization included both lip-movement and sounds made.
Their study confirmed one of the two hypotheses, the “production-deficiency hypothesis,
which stated that children can use strategies they are taught by others to remember information
but cannot produce strategies on their own. The results from this study showed that the younger
children were less likely to show rehearsal or verbalization in the recall task in comparison to
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 6
older children which falls in line with the hypotheses they made concerning the experiment. The
current study aimed to replicate these findings concerning verbalization and rehearsal in
children.
The Effects of Extracurricular Activities
López-Vicente, Garcia-Aymerich, Torrent-Pallicer, Forns, Ibarluzea, Lertxundi,
González, Valera-Gran, Torrent, Dadvand, Vrijheid, and Sunyer (2017) looked at the long-term
effects of physical activity and sedentary levels on working memory performance in early
childhood through a longitudinal study focusing on several age groups. There were two age
groups in this study, a younger subcohort which consisted of participants who started the study at
4 years of age and an older subcohort whose participants started the study at 6 years of age.
Mothers of the children in each subcohort filled out a survey regarding their child’s physical
activity and sedentary levels once they were recruited (when their child was either 4 or 6,
depending on the subcohort in which they were placed). Although the survey questions were not
identical across the regions, they were all formatted to receive a similar answer, which was the
amount of time their child spent on a specific activity. Once answered, the researchers
transformed the categorical variables to continuous variables to ensure that the answers were
uniform in format.
The working memory portion of this experiment took place several years after these
surveys were completed. Children in the younger subcohort completed the task once they
reached 7 years of age and children in the older subcohort completed the task once they reached
14 years of age. The session took 25 minutes and involved child participants completing an n-
back test. In this n-back test “participants have a sequence of stimuli on the computer screen, one
at a time, and they have to respond (hit a button) when the current stimulus matches the one
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 7
presented n steps before” (López-Vicente et al., 2017, p. 36). Participants completed 25 trials and
three levels of difficulty (1-, 2-, and 3-back) for a total of 75 trials.
For their results, the researchers focused on accuracy in the 2-back trials because “it
showed better properties than the 1- and 3-back tasks (e.g., clear age-dependent slope and little
learning effect) in a previous study” (López-Vicente et al., 2017, p. 36). After analyzing their
results, researchers found that there was no significant difference in memory performance in the
younger subcohort between those with lower or higher levels of physical activity. However, they
do say throughout the rest of the article and in their summary that low levels in the younger
subcohort were associated with working memory performance at a later age. In the older
subcohort, they found that lower levels of activity led to a 4.22% decrease in correct responses
on the n-back test. Additionally, high levels of sedentary behaviors were associated with a 5.07%
decrease in correct responses in males in the older subcohort. Although this longitudinal study
helps us better understand the impact physical activity levels can have later on in life, it does not
provide a clear enough pathway from physical activity to increased memory performance. The
physical activity measurements were taken at the beginning of the study and no follow-up
questions were asked to determine if a participant’s physical activity levels had significantly
changed. To find a clearer path between the two variables, I examined Kamijo, Pontifex,
O’Leary, Scudder, Wu, Castelli, and Hillman (2011), who tested the effects of physical activity
intervention on working memory.
In Kamijo et al. (2011), researchers recruited forty-three children and divided them into
two groups: a waitlist control group and a physical activity program group. Children in the
physical activity intervention group spent two hours each day after school participating in
activities that focused on cardiorespiratory fitness or muscle fitness, depending on the day.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 8
Additionally, the children played organizational games that centered around a skill, such as
dribbling. To measure cognitive ability, each participant completed the modified Sternberg task
made by researchers. In this Sternberg task, participants were presented with a set of uppercase
consonants that were either 1, 3, or 5 letters in length. They were then immediately presented
with a lowercase consonant that was flanked on each side with question marks (to correspond
with the number of letters in the initial presentation). Participants then pressed one of two
buttons to indicate if the uppercase version of that letter had been present in the initial
presentation they had just seen of uppercase consonant letters. For example, participants may be
presented with the letters “LSRMK” and then “??r??”, at which point they should press the
button that indicates the “r” was presented before. Participants completed four blocks of 45 trials
of this task.
After analyzing the data for task performance, researchers reported that participants in the
intervention group saw overall significant improvement in their scores on the Sternberg post-test
(p = .002; 58.4% accuracy to 68.5% accuracy), in comparison to the waitlist control group which
saw no improvement in scores (p = .9; 65.6% accuracy to 66.0% accuracy). As expected, there
were significant decreases in response accuracy as participants were presented with trials
containing more letters. Additionally, differences in response accuracy between groups became
smaller as participants completed trials containing 3 (70.4% accuracy for intervention and 68.2%
for control) and 5 letters (61.0% for intervention and 61.1% for waitlist). Interestingly, while
response accuracy increased for the intervention group (66.0% to 74.1%) they decreased for the
waitlist group (72.9% to 68.8%).
It is important to note that in Kamijo et al. (2011) the intervention group did not have
significantly higher response accuracy in the post-test compared to the waitlist group, they just
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 9
had a significant improvement from their pre-test response accuracy scores. Although the
researchers say that “preliminary t-tests were conducted to confirm that there were no significant
differences in response accuracy between groups at pre-test for each letter condition”, the
significant results in post-test for the intervention group would suggest otherwise (Kamijo et al.,
2011, p. 8). Because the post-test results were not significant between the two groups, a
significant difference in their pre-test would provide an explanation for the researchers’ results.
Overall, Kamijo et al. (2011) did show that physical activity intervention can have a direct result
on a child’s ability to perform on a working memory task.
Furthermore, Hsieh, Fung, Tsai, Chang, Huang, and Hung (2018) looked at the
relationship between physical activity and working memory in their experiment in a small group
of children in Taiwan. Hsieh et al. (2018) classified their participants into two groups, high
physical activity (HP) and low physical activity (LP) based on measurements from an
accelerometer. Groups were determined based on the median number of accelerometer counts
per minute (median = 846 counts). Participants were then given a delayed matching test to
evaluate perceptual working memory. This test had two sections which were classified as
delayed and non-delayed. In the delayed condition participants were presented with a rectangle
with a dot inside of it, at one of nine possible positions, on either the right or left of a plus sign in
the middle of the screen. Next a screen with just the plus sign in the middle was shown for 3
seconds before another rectangle appeared. Participants had to determine if the dot was in the
same place as the previous rectangle or if it had moved. In the non-delayed condition, both
rectangles were presented at once and participants had to determine if the dots in each of the
rectangles were in the same position. Their results found that children in the HP group had
higher accuracy rates (F(1,30) = 4.96, p <.05, η
p
2
= 0.61) compared to children in the LP group.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 10
Current Study
This current study examined how extracurricular activities affected the rehearsal strategies a
child uses to remember information presented in a short-term memory recall task. Additionally,
it examined the effect that long-term extracurricular activities can have on short-term memory
performance. It is an extension of the Elliott et al. (2019) study, which is a multi-lab direct
replication of Flavell, Beach, and Chinsky (1966). This study followed the methods section of
the Elliott et al. (2019) study with the addition of a questionnaire completed by parents
concerning their children’s extracurricular activities. Based off of previous studies, I
hypothesized that than an increase in extracurricular activities may lead to better performance on
the short-term memory task. Additionally, I hypothesized that greater extracurricular activities
may cause a child to perform better than another child who is similar in age or slightly older.
Participants
Participants were recruited from various elementary schools located in Baton Rouge,
Louisiana. An email was sent to school principals explaining the project and asking them to
share the signup form with the guardians of children in kindergarten through fifth grade.
Additionally, Dr. Elliott reached out to several colleagues at Louisiana State University who had
children that fell within our desired age range. A total of 57 children agreed to participate in the
study, (24 male, 33 female). Following the in-person session, the survey sent out to guardians
regarding extracurricular activity performance received 29 completed responses. Of those 29
completed responses, 3 were excluded because the children did not participate in the study but a
sibling did, 1 was excluded because the parent filled out the survey twice, and 2 were excluded
because they began the testing portion of the study but were unable to finish or their data were
unusable due to not following directions. Additionally, one parent filled out one survey for her
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 11
twin daughters, but follow-up contact established that both children completed the same
extracurriculars and therefore could be counted twice. In the end, 24 children (13 females, 11
males) who participated in the study and whose parent also completely filled out the survey
regarding extracurriculars were included in the analyses.
Procedure
Testing for this study took place in two different environments. The majority of children
were tested in Dr. Emily Elliott’s lab located on Louisiana State University’s campus in
Audubon Hall. Some children were tested at their local elementary school, during their after
school program, in the school’s library or an empty classroom. Both locations had relatively the
same set up and testing was conducted in the same order and manner in both locations.
Participants sat behind a computer screen with an experimenter on the side of them to facilitate
the computer program used to run the test and explain the study to the participant. For most
trials, there were between one and two coders also in the room sitting a few feet from the
experimenter and participant depending on availability for that day. However, when there was no
coder in the room the computer’s built-in camera was used to record the session. Later on, coders
went through the video and coded the child’s verbalization throughout the test. A minor assent
form was procured from the participant before the test began in addition to the consent form
filled out by the participant’s guardian.
The experimenter and participant sat in chairs behind the computer screen while the test
was conducted. First, the demographic information was entered for that participant which
included: participant number, research group (LSU), age in months, grade in school, and gender
(in that order). The experimenter then explained what the memory recall task would consist of
based on prompts from the computer program. The experimenter explained that several pictures
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 12
would appear in a line on the screen and the participant was expected to memorize, to the best of
their ability, the pictures that were highlighted by an orange color and the order in which they
were highlighted. Participants then went through two practice trials, the first with two pictures
being highlighted and the second with four pictures being highlighted and were asked
immediately to recall the pictures they saw following each trial, by pointing to the pictures in
order. It was emphasized to participants that they should just point to the pictures during the
recall phase, and no mention was made of having them name the pictures for the first two
sections of the memory recall task. Participants were then asked if they had any questions
concerning the practice trials they just did, and if they did not then the experimenter proceeded to
explain the next step.
It was then explained that during some trials they would be asked to wear a pair of
painter’s-taped sunglasses during a “delay” period of fifteen seconds. Participants were given the
opportunity to practice putting the sunglasses on for fifteen seconds to get a feel for how long
they would have to remain on, some chose to practice putting the sunglasses on while others did
not. With the practice trials over and the role of the sunglasses in the delay period explained, the
test began. Each participant experienced all three sections of the memory recall: delayed recall,
immediate recall, and point-and-name recall. The delayed recall involved the presentation of
pictures, wearing the sunglass for fifteen seconds, and then the recall of the pictures in the order
they were seen. The immediate recall followed the same procedure minus wearing the sunglasses
for fifteen seconds. The point-and-name recall mirrored the delayed recall, but during the
presentation and recall phases the participants were asked to point and name the objects that they
saw highlighted and the order they saw them; respectively.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 13
Each section of the memory recall task involved eight trials, two trials for each number
sequence of pictures (two, three, four, and five) and the trials progressed sequentially, meaning
that during the delay trials participants saw two of the two-picture trials first, then the two three-
picture trials, and so on until they finished with the two five-picture trials before heading onto
the next section. Participants experienced either the delay or immediate recall section first, based
on randomization from the computer program, followed by whichever was left from that pair,
and the point-and-name section always last. After the delayed recall section participants were
asked how they remembered the sequence of pictures (see Appendix B for a copy of the
questionnaire). Before each point-and-name section participants were asked to name the seven
pictures one at a time to ensure that the participants had a verbal label for each item (see
Appendix C for a copy of the pictures). Once the point-and-name section was finished
participants were informed that they had finished the test, were allowed to pick out a prize, and
returned to their guardian who was waiting just outside the testing room.
A survey regarding extracurricular activities was emailed out to the parents after
participant testing was finished (see Appendix A for a copy of the questionnaire). It was
determined that the guardians would be able to give the most accurate answers for the
questionnaire in comparison to the participants. This survey was constructed through LSU
Qualtrics and was distributed to guardians in December of 2019. In the survey, guardians were
asked to choose from a list what extracurriculars their child participated in, with an option at the
end for them to write in any activities that may have been left of out the list. After choosing the
activities they then answered questions concerning the amount of time their child had been
involved in each activity such as hours per week and months per year. The second section of the
questionnaire resembled the first but focused on musical instruments that their child practiced.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 14
Results
To analyze the data from this study I utilized a correlation analysis to test the
relationships of our variables (extracurriculars, rehearsal, and working memory; see Table 1).
The goal of this analysis was to determine how extracurriculars affect the amount of rehearsal a
participant engages in, whether increased rehearsal affects accuracy in a short-term memory task,
and if high levels of extracurriculars activities correlated to higher accuracy in the memory task.
Results showed a non-significant negative correlation between verbalization and extracurricular
hours (p = 0.613, Pearson’s r = -0.109). There was also a non-significant negative correlation
between verbalization and serial position score (p = 0.325, Pearson’s r = -0.210). The analysis
showed a significant positive correlation between extracurricular hours and serial position score
(p = 0.013, Pearson’s r = 0.499). Other significant results are between age in months and
extracurricular hours (p = 0.003, Pearson’s r = 0.586) and between age in months and serial
position score (p < 0.001, Pearson’s r = 0.499). There was a non-significant negative correlation
between age in months and mean verbalization score (p = 0.640, Pearson’s r = -0.101). A
mediation analysis was conducted with extracurricular activities acting as the mediator on ages
in months for serial position score (see Table 2). There was a nonsignificant indirect mediation
effect (p = 0.873) and a significant direct mediation effect (p < .001).
Discussion
The first hypothesis for this study was that higher levels of extracurricular activity may
lead to better performance on the short-term memory task. This hypothesis was supported based
on the results of the memory recall task. The correlation analysis showed that as participants
spent more hours in extracurriculars activities their chances of a higher serial position recall
score rose. Although in the proposal for this study I expressed the idea that this correlation would
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 15
be the result of children better rehearsal from their extracurricular activities, the correlation
analysis between extracurricular hours and mean verbalization was non-significant. However, the
insignificant negative trend towards 1 (which indicated clear verbalization during testing trials)
indicated that further testing with larger sample sizes would be a promising future direction.
Additionally, this study derived verbalization as a measure of rehearsal in participants
from Flavell et al. (1966) but did not find the same results as their study (see Table 3). While
Flavell et al. (1966) found that children verbalized more as they aged, this study found no
significant correlation between age and verbalization scores. In fact, the results showed a slight,
non-significant negative correlation between the two variables. Due to the age of the original
study, it is possible that children have learned other, internal rehearsal strategies that are more
effective and have caused the decrease in verbalization scores. Miller et al. (2015) explored two
different types of non-verbal rehearsal, which are cumulative sub-vocal rehearsal and interactive
imagery, in their study. It is possible that children are practicing these internal rehearsal
strategies and are becoming more successful at internalizing rehearsal as they age.
There are other factors that may explain the correlation between extracurricular activity
and serial position recall scores besides rehearsal. Physical activity has been known to have
effects on cognitive performance because of increased blood flow to the brain. One way to test
this difference would be to have an experiment with two different groups of physical activity:
extracurricular activities that involve strategy and regular physical activity, such as running, that
do not involve strategy. Additionally, a demographics survey for parents and guardians to fill out
could be used to account for other variables that may influence outcomes such as socioeconomic
status, access/use of tutoring services, and reading levels outside of schoolwork.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 16
The second hypothesis that greater levels of extracurricular activity may cause a child to
perform better than another child who is similar in age or slightly older is not supported by the
results. The idea behind this hypothesis was that extracurricular activities would again improve
rehearsal in children and therefore help them perform better on a short-term memory recall task.
However, the results of the mediation analysis of extracurricular activities on the relationship
between age and serial position score show otherwise. It is possible that more extracurricular
activities may put a child above other people in their age group but not above or at the same level
as children ahead of them because of other factors involved in a child’s abilities to rehearse and
recall information. The educational tools children are given as they advance through school
could be what set the older children apart in terms of serial position scores, despite having
similar or fewer hours of extracurricular activities. Additionally, higher attention spans and
continuing brain development could play a role.
There are several improvements that could be made for future replications or extensions
of this study. The largest obstacle faced in this study involved the survey portion which would
need to be modified for future usage. There were errors in the collected questionnaire data
because guardians were allowed to write-in their answers for each question after selecting their
child’s extracurricular activities. One possible solution for this would be changing the answer
format to a drop-down list for each question. For example, for a question like, what months out
of the school year did your child participate in an activity?”, there would be a drop-down menu
for the start month and end month.
Another future improvement to this study would be having guardians fill out the survey
while their child is participating in the memory recall task, which would also give them
something to do while their child was being tested. There were some instances where parents
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 17
wanted to be in the room with their children which resulted in possible distractions for the child.
Additionally, having guardians fill out the survey when they bring their child in for testing would
ensure that we would collect a timely and accurate response. There were 57 participants that
were run through the memory recall task and less than half of those had a survey filled out by
their guardian. The survey for this study was sent out anywhere from a few weeks to several
months after the child participated in the recall task, which was detrimental to the response
count. There is a possibility that guardians unknowingly put in extracurricular hours from after
their child participated in the memory recall task but before they received the questionnaire.
Having the survey done immediately upon arrival would likely mean receiving a more accurate
response and being able to sort out any problems guardians may face with the survey in a timely
manner.
Another improvement would be to do further analysis on the different sections of the
memory recall test. Some participants performed very well on certain sections and then
performed poorly on others (See Table 4). Further research could be done into the cause of this
difference and whether it has to do with the difference between sections (such as initial rehearsal
in the point-and-name section compared to the first two sections). By breaking down the
different sections of the memory task, further exploration could also be done on the effects of
different extracurricular activities on these sections. Different extracurricular activities promote
different types of rehearsal and memory recall, which the testing portion of this study does
through its three sections. However, research into this idea required also looking at the
limitations of this study.
The limitations of this study revolved largely around the sample size of this study. Earlier
in this discussion it was mentioned that a future experiment could look at two different types of
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 18
physical activity which were extracurricular activity and “regular” physical activity. A larger
sample size would allow for this to happen, in addition to further distinguishing between types of
extracurricular activities. Different extracurricular activities require different levels of rehearsal
and strategizing and classifying these as higher or lower in terms of rehearsal would be
beneficial for future research. Additionally, a larger sample size would allow for partial
correlations to control for age in months. The sample size of the current study was too small to
successfully complete additional analyses.
In conclusion, the results of this study indicated that children who participate more in
extracurricular activities may have better short-term memory. The results also indicated that an
increase in extracurricular hours does not lead to an increase in verbalization. Finally, the results
of this study indicated that verbalization does not have an impact on short term memory recall
and provided several clear avenues for future research.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 19
References
Elliott, E. M., Morey, C. C., & AuBuchon, A. (provisionally accepted). Registered replication
report of Flavell, Beach, and Chinsky (1966). Advances in Methods and Practices in
Psychological Science.
Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. R., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory
task as a function of age. Child Development,37(2), 283-299. doi:10.2307/1126804
Hsieh, S., Fung, D., Tsai, H., Chang, Y., Huang, C., & Hung, T. (2018). Differences in working
memory as a function of physical activity in children. Neuropsychology,32(7), 797-808.
doi:10.1037/neu0000473
Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., O’Leary, K. C., Scudder, M. R., Wu, C., Castelli, D. M., & Hillman, C.
H. (2011). The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in
preadolescent children. Developmental Science,14(5), 1046-1058. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
7687.2011.01054.x
López-Vicente, M., Garcia-Aymerich, J., Torrent-Pallicer, J., Forns, J., Ibarluzea, J., Lertxundi, N., .
. . Sunyer, J. (2017). Are Early Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviors Related to Working
Memory at 7 and 14 Years of Age? The Journal of Pediatrics,188, 35-41.
doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.05.079
Miller, S., McCulloch, S., & Jarrold, C. (2015). The development of memory maintenance
strategies: Training cumulative rehearsal and interactive imagery in children aged between 5 and
9. Frontiers in Psychology,06. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00524
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 20
Table 1
Correlation Matrix of Extracurricular Hours, Age in Months, Mean Verbalization Scores,
and Serial Position Scores
Ex. Hours
Age in
Months
Mean
Verb.
SP
Score
Ex. Hours
Age in Months
0.586
**
0.003
Mean Verb.
-0.109
-0.101
0.613
0.640
SP Score
0.499
*
0.826
***
-0.210
0.013
< .001
0.325
Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Table 2
Mediation Estimates of Extracurricular Hours
Effect
Estimate
SE
Z
p
Indirect
0.0125
0.0778
0.160
0.873
Direct
0.7609
0.1328
5.729
< .001
Total
0.7734
0.1077
7.179
< .001
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 21
Figure 1:
Histogram and Scatterplot Graphs Among the Four Variables.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 22
Table 3
Number of Ss Showing 1+ Verbalization Instances on Each Segment of Each Subtask.
Segments and Subtasks
Presentation
Delay
Recall
Age
IR
DR
DR
PN
IR
DR
5 (5)
2
2
3
4
3
3
6 (2)
0
0
0
1
2
0
7 (2)
1
2
1
2
2
2
8 (8)
5
5
6
5
7
6
9 (1)
1
1
1
1
1
1
10 (3)
1
3
1
2
2
3
11 (3)
3
3
3
3
3
3
Note. In parentheses beside each age number is the number of participants in that age group.
Table 4
Number of words recalled in the correct serial position by each participant.
Memory Recall Test Section
Participant ID
Delayed
Immediate
Point & Name
Grand Total
107
4
2
9
15
108
5
9
4
18
109
9
14
20
43
111
4
4
7
15
114
3
13
8
24
118
12
8
3
23
200
14
22
12
28
201
27
27
21
75
202
22
25
23
70
203
10
17
12
39
210
7
10
10
27
215
13
20
11
44
302
13
21
16
50
303
12
17
12
41
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 23
304
16
20
15
51
311
21
19
24
64
313
10
9
6
25
314
19
17
20
56
400
28
27
28
83
406
26
20
17
63
411
26
25
28
79
412
23
25
25
73
500
26
19
18
63
510
15
24
16
55
Grand Total
375
431
383
1169
Note. The maximum possible recall score for each participant was 84.
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 24
Appendix A
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 25
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 26
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 27
Appendix B
EXTRACURRICULARS, REHEARSAL, AND STM 28
Appendix C
comb flag pencil
apple moon owl flower